Beyond “Uniqueness”: Habitual Traits in the Context of Cognitive-Communicative Continuity
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.12775/ths.2019.008Keywords
continuity, comparative communication, comparative cognition, language evolution, comparative method, habitual trait, gesture, primate communication, infant pointingAbstract
Abstract. The paper discusses a longstanding and prevailing debate in thefields of comparative psychology and language evolution, namely the problem of continuity of cognitive and communicative abilities. It is suggested that although this problem has been addressed by multiple researchers, it remains a persistent topic of discussion and an important meta-theoretical theme in the field of language evolution. To support this claim, the paper first provides a brief overview of the continuity debate by discussing examples of prominent research work in comparative communication and cognition. It further suggests that the problem of continuity can be partly resolved by focusing on cognitive and behavioural trait distribution both between and within species. Specifically, it is proposed that conceptualising given traits as habitual, rather than human-unique, is informative for understanding evolutionary processes shaping human communicative abilities. Finally, the paper suggests that the debate on continuity would benefit from being placed in a broader framework of evolution of phenotypic plasticity and the role of cognition and behavior in evolutionary processes.
References
Alexander, R. M. (2004). Bipedal animals, and their differences from humans. Journal of Anatomy, 204(5), 321–330.
Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism–How to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(4), 489–504.
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. S. (1991). From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 21(3), 205–226.
Butler, A. B. (2008). Evolution of brains, cognition, and consciousness. Brain Research Bulletin, 75(2–4), 442–449.
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1994). Production and comprehension of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108(4), 307.
Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Bellagamba, F., & Colonnesi, C. (2004). The role of declarative pointing in developing a theory of mind. Infancy, 5(3), 291–308.
Carvalho, S., Biro, D., Cunha, E., Hockings, K., McGrew, W. C., Richmond, B. G., & Matsuzawa, T. (2012). Chimpanzee carrying behaviour and the origins of human bipedality. Current Biology, 22(6), R180–R181.
Clay, Z., Archbold, J., & Zuberbühler, K. (2015). Functional flexibility in wild bonobo vocal behaviour. PeerJ, 3, e1124.
Cochet, H., & Vauclair, J. (2010). Pointing gestures produced by toddlers from 15 to 30 months: Different functions, hand shapes and laterality patterns. Infant Behavior and Development, 33(4), 431–441.
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366.
Crockford, C., Wittig, R. M., Mundry, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2012). Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22(2), 142–146.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London: John Murray.
Day, M. H., & Wickens, E. H. (1980). Laetoli Pliocene hominid footprints and bipedalism. Nature, 286(5771), 385.
de Waal, F. B., & Ferrari, P. F. (2010). Towards a bottom-up perspective on animal and human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5), 201–207.
Douglas, P. H., & Moscovice, L. R. (2015). Pointing and pantomime in wild apes? Female bonobos use referential and iconic gestures to request genito-genital rubbing. Scientific Reports, 5, 13999.
Fitch W. T. (2010). The evolution of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Fitch, W. T. (2012). Evolutionary developmental biology and human language evolution: Constraints on adaptation. Evolutionary Biology, 39(4), 613–637.
Fitch, W. T. (2017). Empirical approaches to the study of language evolution. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 3–33.
Foster, S. A. (2013). Evolution of behavioural phenotypes: Influences of ancestry and expression. Animal Behaviour, 85(5), 1061–1075.
Foster, S. A., Wund, M. A., & Baker, J. A. (2015). Evolutionary influences of plastic behavioral responses upon environmental challenges in an adaptive radiation. Integrative and Comparative Biology, 55(3), 406–417.
Fusco, G. (2001). How many processes are responsible for phenotypic evolution? Evolution & Development, 3(4), 279–286.
Fusco, G., & Minelli, A. (2010). Phenotypic plasticity in development and evolution: Facts and concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1540), 547–556.
Gilbert, S. F., & Epel, D. (2009). Ecological developmental biology: Integrating epigenetics, medicine, and evolution. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.
Halina, M., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2018). The goal of ape pointing. PloS One, 13(4), e0195182.
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59(4), 771–785.
Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579.
Hayama, S., Nakatsukasa, M., & Kunimatsu, Y. (1992). Monkey performance: The development of bipedalism in trained Japanese monkeys. Kaibogaku zasshi. Journal of Anatomy, 67(3), 169–185.
Herculano-Houzel, S. (2011). Scaling of brain metabolism with a fixed energy budget per neuron: Implications for neuronal activity, plasticity and evolution. PloS One, 6(3), e17514.
Hurford, J. R. (2014). Origins of language: A slim guide. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2008). Soft inheritance: Challenging the modern synthesis. Genetics and Molecular Biology, 31(2), 389–395.
Jacob, F. (1977). Evolution and tinkering. Science, 196(4295), 1161–1166.
Kako, E. (1999). Elements of syntax in the systems of three language-trained animals. Animal Learning & Behavior, 27(1), 1–14.
Kay, E. D., & Condon, K. (1987). Skeletal changes in the hindlimbs of bipedal rats. The Anatomical Record, 218(1), 1–4.
Kilian, A., Yaman, S., von Fersen, L., & Güntürkün, O. (2003). A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity. Animal Learning & Behavior, 31(2), 133–142.
Koda, H., Lemasson, A., Oyakawa, C., Pamungkas, J., & Masataka, N. (2013).
Possible role of mother-daughter vocal interactions on the development of species-specific song in gibbons. PLoS One 8(8), e71432.
Kurismaa, A. (2015). On the origins of anticipation as an evolutionary framework: Functional systems perspective. International Journal of General Systems, 44(6), 705–721.
Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2017). The mismeasure of ape social cognition. Animal Cognition. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/
article/10.1007/s10071-017-1119-1
Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Bard, K. A. (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: Epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions
in Psychological Science, 14(4), 185–189.
Leavens, D. A., Racine, T. P., & Hopkins, W. D. (2009). The ontogeny and phylogeny of non-verbal deixis. The Prehistory of Language, 11, 142.
Legerstee, M., & Barillas, Y. (2003). Sharing attention and pointing to objects at 12 months: Is the intentional stance implied? Cognitive Development, 18(1), 91–110.
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A., & de Vos, C. (2011).
A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713.
Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Pointing out new news, old news, and absent referents at 12 months of age. Developmental Science, 10(2), F1–F7.
Lyn, H., Greenfield, P. M., Savage-Rumbaugh, S., Gillespie-Lynch, K., & Hopkins, W. D. (2011). Nonhuman primates do declare! A comparison of declarative symbol and gesture use in two children, two bonobos, and a chimpanzee. Language & Communication, 31(1), 63–74.
Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., & Hopkins, W. D. (2010). The impact of environment on the comprehension of declarative communication in apes. Psychological Science, 21(3), 360–365.
Lyn, H., Russell, J. L., Leavens, D. A., Bard, K. A., Boysen, S. T., Schaeffer, J. A., & Hopkins, W. D. (2014). Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: Methodology matters. Animal Cognition, 17(1), 85–94.
Martins, P. T., & Boeckx, C. (2016). Language evolution: Insisting on making it a mystery or turning it into a problem. In L. Dupuy, A. Grabizna, N. Foudon, & P. Saint-Germier (Eds.), Papers dedicated to anne reboul (pp. 1–8). Lyon: Institut des Sciences Cognitives.
Moss, M. L. (1961). Rotation of the otic capsule in bipedal rats. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 19, 301–307.
Nakatsukasa, M., Hayama, S., & Preuschoft, H. (1995). Postcranial skeleton of a macaque trained for bipedal standing and walking and implications for functional adaptation. Folia Primatologica, 64(1–2), 1–29.
Niemitz, C. (2010). The evolution of the upright posture and gait—A review and a new synthesis. Naturwissenschaften, 97(3), 241–263.
Oller, D. K., Buder, E. H., Ramsdell, H. L., Warlaumont, A. S., Chorna, L., & Bakeman, R. (2013). Functional flexibility of infant vocalization and the emergence of language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(16), 6318–6323.
Penn, D. C., Holyoak, K. J., & Povinelli, D. J. (2008). Darwin’s mistake: Explaining the discontinuity between human and nonhuman minds. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31(2), 109–130.
Pika, S. (2008). Gestures of apes and pre-linguistic human children: Similar or different? First Language, 28(2), 116–140.
Pika, S., Liebal, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Gestural communication of apes. Gesture, 5(1), 41–56.
Pika, S., Wilkinson, R., Kendrick, K. H., & Vernes, S. C. (2018). Taking turns: Bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1880), 20180598.
Povinelli, D. J., Bering, J. M., & Giambrone, S. (2003). Chimpanzee ‘pointing’: Another error of the argument by analogy. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 35–68). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Povinelli, D. J., Reaux, J. E., Bierschwale, D. T., Allain, A. D., & Simon, B. B. (1997). Exploitation of pointing as a referential gesture in young children, but not adolescent chimpanzees. Cognitive Development, 12(4), 423–461.
Premack, D. (2007). Human and animal cognition: Continuity and discontinuity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(35), 13861–13867.
Roth, G., & Dicke, U. (2005). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(5), 250–257.
Scharff, C., & Petri, J. (2011). Evo-devo, deep homology and FoxP2: Implications for the evolution of speech and language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1574), 2124–2140.
Severcov, A. N. (1922). General questions of evolution. Evolution and mentality. In Sobranie sochineniy (Vol. 3). Moscow: USSR Academy of Sciences Publishing.
Shubin, N., Tabin, C., & Carroll, S. (2009). Deep homology and the origins of evolutionary novelty. Nature, 457(7231), 818.
Slijper, E. J. (1942). Biologic anatomical investigations on the bipedal gait and upright posture in mammals, with special reference to a little goat born without forelegs II. Proceedings of the Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie Van Wetenschappen, 45(1/5), 407–415.
Smirnova, A., Zorina, Z., Obozova, T., & Wasserman, E. (2015). Crows spontaneously exhibit analogical reasoning. Current Biology, 25(2), 256–260.
Southgate, V., van Maanen, C., & Csibra, G. (2007). Infant pointing: Communication to cooperate or communication to learn? Child Development, 78(3), 735–740.
Tallerman, M. (2016). Against the emergent view of language evolution. Proceedings of EvoLang XI. Retrieved from http://evolang.org/neworleans/papers/6.html ten Cate, C. (2016). Assessing the uniqueness of language: Animal grammatical abilities take center stage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(1), 91–96.
Tomasello, M. (2006). Why don’t apes point? In N. Enfield & S. Levinson (Eds.), The roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and interaction (pp. 506–524). Oxford: Berg.
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., & Liszkowski, U. (2007). A new look at infant pointing. Child Development, 78(3), 705–722.
Tomasello, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2003). What makes human cognition unique? From individual to shared to collective intentionality. Mind & Language, 18(2), 121–147.
Thompson, R. K., & Oden, D. L. (2000). Categorical perception and conceptual judgments by nonhuman primates: The paleological monkey and the analogical
ape. Cognitive Science, 24(3), 363–396.
Toomela, A. (2010). Biological roots of foresight and mental time travel. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44(2), 97–125.
Ullrich, R., Mittelbach, M., & Liebal, K. (2017). Scala naturae: The impact of historical values on current ‘evolution of language’ discourse. Journal
of Language Evolution, 3(1), 1–12.
Vereecke, E., D’Août, K., van Elsacker, L., de Clercq, D., & Aerts, P. (2005). Functional analysis of the gibbon foot during terrestrial bipedal walking: Plantar pressure distributions and three‐dimensional ground reaction forces. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 128(3), 659–669.
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2005). Developmental plasticity and the origin of species differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(suppl 1), 6543–6549.
Zorina, Z. A., & Smirnova, A. A. (2006) O chem rasskazali “govoryashchie” obez’yany. (Sposobny li vysshie zhivotnye operirovat’ simvolami?) Moscow: Languages of Slavic Cultures.
Zuk, M., Bastiaans, E., Langkilde, T., & Swanger, E. (2014). The role of behaviour in the establishment of novel traits. Animal Behaviour, 92, 333–344.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
Stats
Number of views and downloads: 1002
Number of citations: 1