The pragmatic foundations of communication: An action-oriented model of the origin of language
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.12775/ths-2014-004Keywords
embodied cognition, gestural origin of language, grounding, mirror neurons, pragmaticsAbstract
In this paper we propose an action-oriented model of language origins that is compatible with the embodied perspective of mind. Specifically, we maintain that a crucial issue for the investigation of the origin of human communication is how language is grounded in the context. In support of such a proposal, we maintain that: a) the grounding of language is tied to a specific aspect of the embodied mind, namely the aspect that emphasizes the embedded character of cognition; b) the emphasis posed on this particular character fits well with a model of language origins founded on gesture. The connection of embedded cognition and the gestural model allows us to propose an explanation of language origin capable of overcoming the problems in the classical symbolic framework of cognitive sciences.
References
Arbib, M. (2005). From monkey-like action recognition to human language: An evolutionary framework for neurolinguistics. Behavioral and brain sciences 28.2: 105–124.
Aziz-Zadeh, L. and R. B. Ivry. (2009). The Human Mirror Neuron System and Embodied Representations. In D. Sternad (ed.), Progress in Motor Contro,l 355–376. Berlin: Springer.
Bak, T., D. O'Donovan, J. Xuereb, S. Boniface and J. Hodges. (2001). Selective impairment of verb processing associated with pathological changes in Brodmann areas 44 and 45 in the motor neurone disease–dementia–aphasia syndrome. Brain 124.1: 103–120.
Bak, T., D. Yancopoulou, P. Nestor, J. Xuereb, M. Spillantini, F. Pulvermüller and J. Hodges. (2006). Clinical, imaging and pathological correlates of a hereditary deficit in verb and action processing. Brain, 129.2: 321–332.
Barsalou, L. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–645.
Beer, R. (2014). Dynamical systems and embedded cognition. In K. Frankish and W. M. Ramsey (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Artificial Intelligence, 128–150. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berwick R., F. Angela, N. Chomsky and J. Bolhuis. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in cognitive sciences 17.2: 89–98.
Binkofski, F. and G. Buccino. (2004). Motor functions of the Broca’s region. Brain and Language 89.2: 362–369.
Bolhuis J., I. Tattersall, N. Chomsky and R. Berwick. (2014). How could language have evolved? http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001934. DOA: 28 Dec. 2014.
Botha, R. (2003). Unravelling the Evolution of Language. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Broca, P. (1861). Perte de la parole, ramollissement chronique et destruction partielle du lob antérieur gauche du cerveau [Loss of Speech, Chronic Softening and Partial Destruction of the Anterior Left Lobe of the Brain]. Bullett. Soc. Anthropol. 2: 235–238.
Calvin, W. and D. Bickerton. (2000). Lingua ex Machina: Reconciling Darwin and Chomsky with the human brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Cantwell Smith, B. (1999). Situatedness/Embeddedness. In R. Wilson and F. Keil (eds.), The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, 769–771. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Cheney, D. and R. Seyfarth. (2010). Primate communication and human language: continuities and discontinuities. In P. Kappeler and J. Silk (eds.), Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origin of Human Universals, 283–298. Heidelberg: Springer.
Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (2005). Language and Mind (Third edition). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chomsky, N. (2010). Some simple evo-devo theses: how true might they be for language? In R. Larson, V. Déprez and H. Yamakido (eds.), The Evolution of Human Language. Biolinguistics Per¬spectives, 45–62. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Chow, Ho M., R. Mar, Y. Xu, S. Liu, S. Wagage and A. Braun. (2014). Embodied comprehension of stories: interactions between language regions and modality-specific neural systems. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 26.2: 279–295.
Christiansen, M. and N. Chater. (2008). Language as shaped by the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 31.5: 489–509.
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Clay, Z. and Zuberbühler, K. (2014). Vocal communication and social awareness in chimpanzees and bonobos. In D. Dor, C. Knight and J. Lewis (eds.), The Social Origins of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Corballis, M. (2010). Mirror neurons and the evolution of language. Brain and language 112.1: 25–35.
Corballis, M. (2011). The Recursive Mind. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Corballis, M. (2013). Toward a Darwinian perspective on language evolution. In Y. Coello and A. Bartolo (eds.), Language and Action in Cognitive Neuroscience, 33–58. London and New York: Psychogy Press.
Darwin, C. (1859). The Descent of Man; and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: Murray.
Di Pellegrino, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese and G. Rizzolatti. (1992). Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study. Experimental brain research 91.1: 176–180.
Embick, D., A. Marantz, Y. Miyashita, W. O’Neil and K. Sakai. (2000). A syntactic specialization for Broca’s area. PNAS 97.11: 6150–6154.
Engel, A., A. Maye, M. Kurthen and P. König. (2013). Where's the action? The pragmatic turn in cognitive science. Trends in cognitive sciences 17.5: 202–209.
Evans, V. (2014). The Language Myth. Why Language Is Not an Instinct. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Evans, N. and S. C. Levinson. (2009). The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32.5: 429–448.
Ferretti, F. (2007). Perché non siamo speciali [Why we are not special]. Roma and Bari: Laterza.
Ferretti, F. (2013). Navigation, Discourse and the Origin of Language. In P. Brambilla and A. Marini (eds.), Brain evolution, language, and psychopathology in schizophrenia, 22–32. New York: Routledge.
Ferretti, F. and I. Adornetti. (2014). Against linguistic Cartesianism: Toward a naturalistic model of human language origins and functioning. Language & Communication 37: 29–39.
Fodor, J. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Fodor, J. (1980). Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and brain sciences 3.1: 63–73.
Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of the Mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (1987). Psychosemantics: The Problem of Meaning in the Philosophy of Mind. London and Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (2001). The Mind Doesn’t Work that Way. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Fodor, J. (2008). Against Darwinism. Mind & Language 23.1: 1–24.
Fodor, J. and Z. Pylyshyn. (1988). Connectionism and cognitive architecture: a critical analysis. Cognition 28.1: 3–71.
Fogassi, L. and P. F. Ferrari. (2012). Cortical motor organization, mirror neurons, end embodied language: an evolutionary perspective. Biolinguistics 6.3–4: 308–337.
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.
Gallese, V., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi and G. Rizzolatti. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119.2: 593–609.
Gallese, V., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi and G. Rizzolatti. (2002). Action representation and the inferior parietal lobule. In W. Prinz and B. Hommel (eds.), Common Mechanisms in Perception and Action: Attention and Performance Vol. XIX, 334–355. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Gallese, V. and G. Lakoff. (2005). The brain's concepts: The role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge. Cognitive neuropsychology 22.3–4: 455–479.
Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Cooperation and the Evolution of Symbolic Communication. In K. Oller and U. Griebel (eds.), Evolution of Communications Systems: A Comparative Approach, 237–256. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Gärdenfors, P. (2014). The Evolution of Sentential Structure. Humana.Mente – Journal of Philosophical Studies 27.
Gentilucci, M. and M. Corballis. (2006). From manual gesture to speech: A gradual transition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30.7: 949–960.
Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Glenberg, A. and V. Gallese. (2012). Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production. Cortex 48.7: 905–922.
Glenberg, A. and M. Kaschak. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic bulletin & review 9.3: 558–565.
Grafton, S., M. Arbib, L. Fadiga and G. Rizzolatti. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 2. Observation compared with imagination. Experimental Brain Research 112: 103–111.
Grice, H. P. (1968). “Utterer’s meaning, sentence-meaning, and word-meaning.” Foundation of Language 4.3: 225–242.
Hammerschmidt, K. and J. Fischer. (2008). Constraints in primate vocal production. In U. Griebel and K. Oller (eds.), The Evolution of Communicative Creativity: From Fixed Signals to Contextual Flexibility, 93–119. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Happé, F. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition 48.2: 101–119.
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D 42: 335–346.
Hauser M., C. Yang, R. Berwick, I. Tattersall, M. Ryan, J. Watumull, N. Chomsky and R. Lewontin. (2014). The mystery of language evolution. Frontiers in Psychology 5. http://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00401.
Kurby, C. and J. Zacks. (2013). The activation of modality-specific representations during discourse processing. Brain and language 126.3: 338–349.
Lakoff, G. and M. Johnson. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
Meteyard, L., S. Cuadrado, B. Bahrami and G. Vigliocco. (2012). Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics. Cortex 48.7: 788–804.
Mukamel, R., A. Ekstrom, J. Kaplan, M. Iacoboni and I. Fried. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology 20.8: 750–756.
Newell, A. (1980). “Physical symbol systems.” Cognitive Science 4: 135–183.
Origgi G. and D. Sperber. (2000). Evolution, communication and the proper function of language. In P. Carruthers and A. Chamberlain (eds.), Evolution and the Human Mind: Language, Modularity and Social Cognition, 140–169. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Pexman P., K. Rostad, C. McMorris, E. Climie, J. Stowkowy and M. Glenwright. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of autism and developmental disorders 41.8: 1097–1112.
Pinker, S. and P. Bloom. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13.4: 707–784.
Ploog, D. (2002). Is the neural basis of vocalization different in nonhuman primates and Homo sapiens? In T. J. Crow (ed.), The Speciation of Modern Homo Sapiens, 121–135. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Pollick, A. and F. de Waal. (2007). Ape gestures and language evolution. PNAS 104.19: 8184–8189.
Pouw, W., J. de Nooijer, T. van Gog, R. A. Zwaan and F. Paas. (2014). Toward a more embedded/extended perspective on the cognitive function of gestures. Frontiers in psychology 5: 359.
Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6.7: 576–582.
Pylyshyn, Z. (1985). Computation and Cognition: Toward a Foundation for Cognitive Science. London: MIT Press.
Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi and V. Gallese. (2000). Cortical mechanisms subserving object grasping and action recognition: a new view on the cortical motor functions. In M. S. Gazzaniga (ed.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 539–552. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Rizzolatti, G. and M. Arbib. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in neurosciences 21.5: 188–194.
Rizzolatti, G. And C. Sinigaglia. (2008). Mirrors in the brain: How our minds share actions and emotions. Cambridge: Oxford University Press.
Shannon, C. and W. Weaver. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. Urbana: The University of Illinois Press.
Shapiro, L. A. (2010). Embodied Cognition. New York: Routledge.
Sperber, D. and D. Wilson. (1986/1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Sperber, D. and D. Wilson. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind & Language 17.1–2: 3–23.
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Tomasello, M. (2009). Universal Grammar is dead. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32.5: 470–471.
Tomasello, M. and J. Call. (2007). Ape gestures and the origins of language. In J. Call and M. Tomasello (eds.) The Gestural Communication of Apes and Monkeys, 221–239. London: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Tomasello, M., J. Call, K. Nagell, R. Olguin and M. Carpenter. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 37: 137–154.
Turing, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind 49: 433–460.
Varela, F., E. Thompson and E. Rosch. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic bulletin & review 9.4: 625–636.
Zlatev, J. (2014). Bodily mimesis and the transition to speech. In M. Pina and N. Gontier (eds.), The Evolution of Social Communication in Primates, 165-178. Berlin: Springer.
Downloads
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
Stats
Number of views and downloads: 654
Number of citations: 0